nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
Adaptation to other people’s eye gaze reflects habituation of high-level perceptual representations
|
Palmer, Colin J. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 82-90 |
artikel |
2 |
ANCHORING is amodal: Evidence from a signed language
|
Andan, Qatherine |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 279-283 |
artikel |
3 |
Attention capture is temporally stable: Evidence from mixed-model correlations
|
Weichselbaum, Hanna |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 206-224 |
artikel |
4 |
Cognitive development attenuates audiovisual distraction and promotes the selection of task-relevant perceptual saliency during visual search on complex scenes
|
Cavallina, Clarissa |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 91-98 |
artikel |
5 |
Communicative intent modulates production and comprehension of actions and gestures: A Kinect study
|
Trujillo, James P. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 38-51 |
artikel |
6 |
Context mitigates crowding: Peripheral object recognition in real-world images
|
Wijntjes, Maarten W.A. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 158-164 |
artikel |
7 |
Editorial Board
|
|
|
|
180 |
C |
p. ii |
artikel |
8 |
Event segmentation: Cross-linguistic differences in verbal and non-verbal tasks
|
Gerwien, Johannes |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 225-237 |
artikel |
9 |
Expectation affects learning and modulates memory experience at retrieval
|
Kafkas, Alex |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 123-134 |
artikel |
10 |
Grounding the neurobiology of language in first principles: The necessity of non-language-centric explanations for language comprehension
|
Hasson, Uri |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 135-157 |
artikel |
11 |
Identifying others’ informative intentions from movement kinematics
|
McEllin, Luke |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 246-258 |
artikel |
12 |
Intention, attention and long-term memory for visual scenes: It all depends on the scenes
|
Evans, Karla K. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 24-37 |
artikel |
13 |
Intuitive statistical inferences in chimpanzees and humans follow Weber’s law
|
Eckert, Johanna |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 99-107 |
artikel |
14 |
I remember emotional content better, but I’m struggling to remember who said it!
|
Le Bigot, Ludovic |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 52-58 |
artikel |
15 |
Learning to measure through action and gesture: Children’s prior knowledge matters
|
Congdon, Eliza L. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 182-190 |
artikel |
16 |
Meaning before order: Cardinal principle knowledge predicts improvement in understanding the successor principle and exact ordering
|
Spaepen, Elizabet |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 59-81 |
artikel |
17 |
Mental states modulate gaze following, but not automatically
|
Kuhn, Gustav |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 1-9 |
artikel |
18 |
Mnemonic accessibility affects statement believability: The effect of listening to others selectively practicing beliefs
|
Vlasceanu, Madalina |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 238-245 |
artikel |
19 |
Motor simulation of multiple observed actions
|
Cracco, Emiel |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 200-205 |
artikel |
20 |
Not all those who wander are lost: Spatial exploration patterns and their relationship to gender and spatial memory
|
Gagnon, Kyle T. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 108-117 |
artikel |
21 |
Subconscious processing reveals dissociable contextual modulations of visual size perception
|
Chen, Lihong |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 259-267 |
artikel |
22 |
The latent structure of spatial skill: A test of the 2 × 2 typology
|
Mix, Kelly S. |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 268-278 |
artikel |
23 |
The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds’ performance on a nontraditional false-belief task
|
Roby, Erin |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 10-23 |
artikel |
24 |
Typical visual-field locations facilitate access to awareness for everyday objects
|
Kaiser, Daniel |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 118-122 |
artikel |
25 |
Unconscious memory suppression
|
Salvador, Alexandre |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 191-199 |
artikel |
26 |
When do circumstances excuse? Moral prejudices and beliefs about the true self drive preferences for agency-minimizing explanations
|
Cullen, Simon |
|
|
180 |
C |
p. 165-181 |
artikel |