nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
Attention allocation in complementary joint action: How joint goals affect spatial orienting
|
Schmitz, Laura |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1574-1593 |
artikel |
2 |
Attentional suppression of dynamic versus static salient distractors
|
Adams, Owen J. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1-14 |
artikel |
3 |
Attention in redundancy masking
|
Yildirim-Keles, Fazilet Zeynep |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1-14 |
artikel |
4 |
Audiovisual perception of interrupted speech by nonnative listeners
|
Yang, Jing |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1763-1776 |
artikel |
5 |
Bayesian analysis on missing visual information and object complexity on visual search for object orientation and object identity
|
Nguyen, Rachel T. T. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1560-1573 |
artikel |
6 |
Choice enhances touch pleasantness
|
Gorman, Lenka |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1709-1723 |
artikel |
7 |
Correction to: Feature-based attentional control for distractor suppression
|
Kim, Sunghyun |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1833 |
artikel |
8 |
Effects of goal-setting on sustained attention and attention lapses
|
Strayer, Deanna L. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1594-1608 |
artikel |
9 |
Enhancing rhythmic temporal expectations: The dominance of auditory modality under spatial uncertainty
|
Attout, Lucie |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1681-1693 |
artikel |
10 |
Individual differences in emotion-induced blindness: Are they reliable and what do they measure?
|
Edwards, Mark |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1-15 |
artikel |
11 |
Individual differences in the use of top-down versus bottom-up cues to resolve phonetic ambiguity
|
Crinnion, Anne Marie |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1724-1734 |
artikel |
12 |
Intermixed levels of visual search difficulty produce asymmetric probability learning
|
Won, Bo-Yeong |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1545-1559 |
artikel |
13 |
Multiple visual items can be simultaneously compared with target templates in memory
|
Zheng, Yujie |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1641-1652 |
artikel |
14 |
New task–new results? How the gaze cone is influenced by the method of measurement
|
Linke, Linda |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1800-1815 |
artikel |
15 |
Object-based suppression in target search but not in distractor inhibition
|
Jeong, Jiyoon |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1-27 |
artikel |
16 |
On the relationship between spatial attention and semantics in the context of a Stroop paradigm
|
Besner, Derek |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1521-1530 |
artikel |
17 |
Perception of temporal structure in speech is influenced by body movement and individual beat perception ability
|
Rathcke, Tamara |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1746-1762 |
artikel |
18 |
Peripheral vision contributes to implicit attentional learning: Findings from the “mouse-eye” paradigm
|
Chen, Chen |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1621-1640 |
artikel |
19 |
Phonological neighbors cooperate during spoken-sentence processing: Evidence from a nonword detection task
|
Dufour, Sophie |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1735-1745 |
artikel |
20 |
Repulsion bias is insensitive to spatial attention, yet expands during active working memory maintenance
|
Sun, Mengdan |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1653-1667 |
artikel |
21 |
Retinal eccentricity modulates saliency-driven but not relevance-driven visual selection
|
Donk, Mieke |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1609-1620 |
artikel |
22 |
Task-order control in dual-tasks: Only marginal interactions between conflict at lower levels and higher processes of task organization
|
Koob, Valentin |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1-23 |
artikel |
23 |
Terms of debate: Consensus definitions to guide the scientific discourse on visual distraction
|
Liesefeld, Heinrich R. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1445-1472 |
artikel |
24 |
The addition of a spatial auditory cue improves spatial updating in a virtual reality navigation task
|
Shayman, Corey S. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1473-1479 |
artikel |
25 |
The interplay between spatial and non-spatial grouping cues over approximate number perception
|
Adriano, Andrea |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1668-1680 |
artikel |
26 |
The who and the where: Attention to identities and locations in groups
|
Ma, Helen L. |
|
|
86 |
5 |
p. 1816-1832 |
artikel |