nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
Action does not drive visual biases in peri-tool space
|
McManus, Robert |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 525-535 |
artikel |
2 |
Adapting attentional control settings in a shape-changing environment
|
Mu, Yunyun |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 404-421 |
artikel |
3 |
An updating-based working memory load alters the dynamics of eye movements but not their spatial extent during free viewing of natural scenes
|
Wyche, Nicholas J. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 503-524 |
artikel |
4 |
Changes in attentional breadth scale with the demands of Kanizsa-figure object completion–evidence from pupillometry
|
Nowack, Leonie |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 439-456 |
artikel |
5 |
Comparing explicit and implicit ensemble perception: 3 stimulus variables and 3 presentation modes
|
Khayat, Noam |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 482-502 |
artikel |
6 |
Contribution of speech rhythm to understanding speech in noisy conditions: Further test of a selective entrainment hypothesis
|
Smith, Toni M. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 627-642 |
artikel |
7 |
Explicit and implicit memory for the QWERTY keyboard: the role of motor simulation and deictic gestures
|
Ianì, Francesco |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 602-615 |
artikel |
8 |
Eye pupil signals life motion perception
|
Cheng, Yuhui |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 579-586 |
artikel |
9 |
Four fundamental dimensions underlie the perception of human actions
|
Vinton, Laura C. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 536-558 |
artikel |
10 |
Holistic processing is modulated by the probability that parts contain task-congruent information
|
Curby, Kim M. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 471-481 |
artikel |
11 |
How state anxious individuals estimate time retrospectively: The mediating effect of memory bias
|
Liu, Jingyuan |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 616-626 |
artikel |
12 |
Linear perspective cues have a greater effect on the perceptual rescaling of distant stimuli than textures in the virtual environment
|
Yildiz, Gizem Y. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 653-665 |
artikel |
13 |
Listening to trees in the forest: Attentional set influences how semantic and acoustic factors interact in auditory perception
|
Dudarev, Veronica |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 381-391 |
artikel |
14 |
Outlier rejection in the process of pooling
|
Lee, Mincheol |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 666-679 |
artikel |
15 |
Reward-based modulation of task-switching performance: a diffusion model analysis
|
Weber, Timo |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 680-706 |
artikel |
16 |
Search mode, not the attentional window, determines the magnitude of attentional capture
|
Kerzel, Dirk |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 457-470 |
artikel |
17 |
Self-initiation enhances perceptual processing of auditory stimuli in an online study
|
Kiepe, Fabian |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 587-601 |
artikel |
18 |
Severe processing capacity limits for sub-lexical features of letter strings
|
Campbell, Maya |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 643-652 |
artikel |
19 |
Spatiotemporal jump detection during continuous film viewing: Insights from a flicker paradigm
|
Upadhyayula, Aditya |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 559-566 |
artikel |
20 |
The effects of eccentricity on attentional capture
|
van Heusden, Elle |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 422-438 |
artikel |
21 |
Think twice: Re-assessing confidence improves visual metacognition
|
Elosegi, Patxi |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 373-380 |
artikel |
22 |
Time and visual-spatial illusions: Evidence for cross-dimensional interference between duration and illusory size
|
Bratzke, Daniel |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 567-578 |
artikel |
23 |
Transient attention does not alter the eccentricity effect in estimation of duration
|
Krug, Alina |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 392-403 |
artikel |
24 |
Visual fixations during processing of time-compressed audiovisual presentations
|
Perez, Nicole D. |
|
|
86 |
2 |
p. 367-372 |
artikel |