nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
Age differences in the use of positive and negative cues to filter distracting information from working memory
|
Torres, Rosa E. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1207-1218 |
artikel |
2 |
Attentional suppression is in place before display onset
|
Huang, Changrun |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1012-1020 |
artikel |
3 |
Auditory scene context facilitates visual recognition of objects in consistent visual scenes
|
Niimi, Ryosuke |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1267-1275 |
artikel |
4 |
Combining social cues in attention: Looking at gaze, head, and pointing cues
|
Lu, Zhifan |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1021-1033 |
artikel |
5 |
Distractor-evoked deviation in saccade direction suggests an asymmetric representation of the upper and lower visual fields on oculomotor maps
|
Huang, Zehao |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1150-1158 |
artikel |
6 |
Does optic flow provide information about actions?
|
Masoner, Hannah L. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1287-1303 |
artikel |
7 |
Effects of internally directed cognition on smooth pursuit eye movements: A systematic examination of perceptual decoupling
|
Korda, Živa |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1159-1178 |
artikel |
8 |
Familiarity influences visual detection in a task that does not require explicit recognition
|
Yang, Pei-Ling |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1127-1149 |
artikel |
9 |
Hand movements influence the perception of time in a prediction motion task
|
Li, Xuening |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1276-1286 |
artikel |
10 |
Insights into the perceptual moment theory: Experimental evidence from simultaneity judgment
|
Lahkar, Ritu |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1199-1206 |
artikel |
11 |
More than a feeling: The emotional attentional blink relies on non-emotional “pop out,” but is weak compared to the attentional blink
|
Santacroce, Lindsay A. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1034-1053 |
artikel |
12 |
No evidence for spatial suppression due to across-trial distractor learning in visual search
|
Li, Ai-Su |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1088-1105 |
artikel |
13 |
Pareidolic faces receive prioritized attention in the dot-probe task
|
Jakobsen, Krisztina V. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1106-1126 |
artikel |
14 |
Perceiving multiple properties of a single person-probe-surface system
|
Wagman, Jeffrey B. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1317-1334 |
artikel |
15 |
Predictability reduces event file retrieval
|
Schmalbrock, Philip |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1073-1087 |
artikel |
16 |
Response assignment influences visual recognition
|
Kang, Mengxue |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1179-1198 |
artikel |
17 |
Sanford’s L dissected: A partial replication and extension of Cai et al. (2017)
|
Landwehr, Klaus |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1304-1316 |
artikel |
18 |
Semantic priming from McGurk words: Priming depends on perception
|
Dorsi, Josh |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1219-1237 |
artikel |
19 |
Slow tempo music preserves attentional efficiency in young children
|
Quan, Yixue |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 978-984 |
artikel |
20 |
Statistical learning of spatiotemporal regularities dynamically guides visual attention across space
|
Xu, Zhenzhen |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1054-1072 |
artikel |
21 |
Stimulus–response congruency effects depend on quality of perceptual evidence: A diffusion model account
|
Tomkins, Blaine |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1335-1354 |
artikel |
22 |
The intrinsic variance of beauty judgment
|
Pombo, Maria |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1355-1373 |
artikel |
23 |
The relative importance of local contingencies and global biases for statistical learning
|
Sewell, Isabella J. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 961-967 |
artikel |
24 |
The role of spatial information in an approximate cross-modal number matching task
|
Ziegler, Marco Carlo |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1253-1266 |
artikel |
25 |
The unnoticed zoo: Inattentional deafness to animal sounds in music
|
Utz, Sandra |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 1238-1252 |
artikel |
26 |
Top-down knowledge surpasses selection history in influencing attentional guidance
|
Grüner, Markus |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 985-1011 |
artikel |
27 |
Visual search and childhood vision impairment: A GAMLSS-oriented multiverse analysis approach
|
Constable, Paul A. |
|
|
85 |
4 |
p. 968-977 |
artikel |