nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
Binding object features to locations: Does the “spatial congruency bias” update with object movement?
|
Bapat, Avni N. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1682-1694 |
artikel |
2 |
Categorical templates are more useful when features are consistent: Evidence from eye movements during search for societally important vehicles
|
Hout, Michael C. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1578-1592 |
artikel |
3 |
Comparing visual search and eye movements in bilinguals and monolinguals
|
Ratiu, Ileana |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1695-1725 |
artikel |
4 |
Conditional control in visual selection
|
van Zoest, Wieske |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1555-1572 |
artikel |
5 |
Does seeing an Asian face make speech sound more accented?
|
Zheng, Yi |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1841-1859 |
artikel |
6 |
Exploratory behaviors and recalibration: What processes are shared between functionally similar affordances?
|
Franchak, John M. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1816-1829 |
artikel |
7 |
Identity and semantic negative priming in rapid serial visual presentation streams
|
Li, Lin |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1755-1776 |
artikel |
8 |
Judgments of differences and ratios of subjective heaviness
|
Masin, Sergio Cesare |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1860-1869 |
artikel |
9 |
Looking sharp: Becoming a search template boosts precision and stability in visual working memory
|
Rajsic, Jason |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1643-1651 |
artikel |
10 |
Measuring and modeling salience with the theory of visual attention
|
Krüger, Alexander |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1593-1614 |
artikel |
11 |
Miss it and miss out: Counterproductive nonspatial attentional capture by task-irrelevant, value-related stimuli
|
Le Pelley, Mike E. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1628-1642 |
artikel |
12 |
Modeling cognitive load effects of conversation between a passenger and driver
|
Tillman, Gabriel |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1795-1803 |
artikel |
13 |
Natural scenes can be identified as rapidly as individual features
|
Howe, Piers D. L. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1674-1681 |
artikel |
14 |
Sustained attention to a predictable, unengaging Go/No-Go task shows ongoing development between 6 and 11 years
|
Lewis, Frances C. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1726-1741 |
artikel |
15 |
The action effect: Support for the biased competition hypothesis
|
Huffman, Greg |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1804-1815 |
artikel |
16 |
The effects of Kanizsa contours on temporal integration and attention in rapid serial visual presentation
|
Karabay, Aytaç |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1742-1754 |
artikel |
17 |
The impact of category structure and training methodology on learning and generalizing within-category representations
|
Ell, Shawn W. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1777-1794 |
artikel |
18 |
The influence of attention on value integration
|
Kunar, Melina A. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1615-1627 |
artikel |
19 |
The role of spatial frequency in emotional face classification
|
Jennings, Ben J. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1573-1577 |
artikel |
20 |
The whereabouts of visual attention: Involuntary attentional bias toward the default gaze direction
|
Nakashima, Ryoichi |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1666-1673 |
artikel |
21 |
Variation in the use of cues to guide visual working memory
|
Robison, Matthew K. |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1652-1665 |
artikel |
22 |
When kinesthetic information is neglected in learning a Novel bimanual rhythmic coordination
|
Zhu, Qin |
|
2017 |
79 |
6 |
p. 1830-1840 |
artikel |