nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
A model to study orienting responses in zebrafish, and applications towards the emotion–cognition interaction
|
do Nascimento, Bianca Gomes |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 965-972 |
artikel |
2 |
A novel, short and easy-to-perform method to evaluate newborns’ social olfactory preferences
|
André, Vanessa |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 843-850 |
artikel |
3 |
Claw-in-the-door: pigeons, like humans, display the foot-in-the-door effect
|
Bartonicek, A. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 893-900 |
artikel |
4 |
Correction to: Effects of reward magnitude and training frequency on the learning rates and memory retention of the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni
|
Heinrich, Dennis D. U. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 951-952 |
artikel |
5 |
Effect of environmental exposure to a maternally-learned odorant on anxiety-like behaviors at weaning in mice
|
Dewaele, Aurélie |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 881-891 |
artikel |
6 |
Effect of previous fighting on the dynamic of agonistic encounters in zebrafish males
|
Cavallino, Luciano |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 999-1006 |
artikel |
7 |
Effect of shared information and owner behavior on showing in dogs (Canis familiaris)
|
Henschel, Melanie |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 1019-1034 |
artikel |
8 |
Effects of reward magnitude and training frequency on the learning rates and memory retention of the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni
|
Heinrich, Dennis D. U. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 939-949 |
artikel |
9 |
Estimating the heritability of cognitive traits across dog breeds reveals highly heritable inhibitory control and communication factors
|
Gnanadesikan, Gitanjali E. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 953-964 |
artikel |
10 |
Is consonance attractive to budgerigars? No evidence from a place preference study
|
Wagner, Bernhard |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 973-987 |
artikel |
11 |
Movement and vocal intonation together evoke social referencing in companion dogs when confronted with a suspicious stranger
|
Salamon, A. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 913-924 |
artikel |
12 |
Multimodal signaling in the visuo-acoustic mismatch paradigm: similarities between dogs and children in the communicative approach
|
Scandurra, Anna |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 833-841 |
artikel |
13 |
Post-event misinformation effects in a language-trained chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
|
James, Brielle T. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 861-869 |
artikel |
14 |
Predictors of individual variation in reversal learning performance in three-spined sticklebacks
|
Bensky, Miles K. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 925-938 |
artikel |
15 |
Righting behaviour in the European pond turtle (Emys orbicularis): relations between behavioural and morphological lateralization
|
Pellitteri-Rosa, Daniele |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 989-998 |
artikel |
16 |
Seeing red? Colour biases of foraging birds are context dependent
|
Teichmann, Marianne |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 1007-1018 |
artikel |
17 |
The relationship between monoaminergic gene expression, learning, and optimism in red junglefowl chicks
|
Boddington, Robert |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 901-911 |
artikel |
18 |
The white-eared opossum failed to understand the parallel strings task: studying a primitive mammal under natural conditions
|
Albert, Tatiani G. |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 871-880 |
artikel |
19 |
Unwilling or willing but unable: can horses interpret human actions as goal directed?
|
Trösch, Miléna |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 1035-1040 |
artikel |
20 |
Visual timing abilities of a harbour seal (Phoca vitulina) and a South African fur seal (Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus) for sub- and supra-second time intervals
|
Heinrich, Tamara |
|
|
23 |
5 |
p. 851-859 |
artikel |