nr |
titel |
auteur |
tijdschrift |
jaar |
jaarg. |
afl. |
pagina('s) |
type |
1 |
A chimpanzee by any other name: The contributions of utterance context and information density on word choice
|
Jacobs, Cassandra L. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
2 |
Attention to number requires magnitude-specific inhibition
|
Viarouge, Arnaud |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
3 |
Auditory cognitive aging in amateur singers and non-singers
|
Tremblay, Pascale |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
4 |
Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience
|
Leach, Stefan |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
5 |
Computational underpinnings of partisan information processing biases and associations with depth of cognitive reasoning
|
Derreumaux, Yrian |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
6 |
Contextual modulation of appearance-trait learning
|
Over, Harriet |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
7 |
Corrigendum to “When forgetting fosters learning: A neural network model for statistical learning” [Cognition (2021) 104621]
|
Endress, Ansgar D. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
8 |
Editorial Board
|
|
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
9 |
Emotion is perceived accurately from isolated body parts, especially hands
|
Blythe, Ellen |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
10 |
Flexible use of confidence to guide advice requests
|
Carlebach, Nomi |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
11 |
Hebbian, correlational learning provides a memory-less mechanism for Statistical Learning irrespective of implementational choices: Reply to Tovar and Westermann (2022)
|
Endress, Ansgar D. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
12 |
How do humans want causes to combine their effects? The role of analytically-defined causal invariance for generalizable causal knowledge
|
Bye, Jeffrey K. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
13 |
How do we describe other people from voices and faces?
|
Lavan, Nadine |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
14 |
How infants' utterances grow: A probabilistic account of early language development
|
Xu, Qihui |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
15 |
Linked auditory and motor patterns in the improvisation vocabulary of an artist-level jazz pianist
|
Norgaard, Martin |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
16 |
Litmus test of rich episodic representations: Context-induced false recognition
|
Szőllősi, Ágnes |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
17 |
Memory failure predicts belief regression after the correction of misinformation
|
Swire-Thompson, Briony |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
18 |
Minimally counterintuitive stimuli trigger greater curiosity than merely improbable stimuli
|
Lewry, Casey |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
19 |
Modeling task effects in human reading with neural network-based attention
|
Hahn, Michael |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
20 |
Modeling the left digit effect in adult number line estimation
|
Patalano, Andrea L. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
21 |
Neural evidence suggests phonological acceptability judgments reflect similarity, not constraint evaluation
|
Avcu, Enes |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
22 |
No need to forget, just keep the balance: Hebbian neural networks for statistical learning
|
Tovar, Ángel Eugenio |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
23 |
Perceptual bias contextualized in visually ambiguous stimuli
|
Esposito, Antonino |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
24 |
Rule is a dual character concept
|
da Franca Couto Fernandes de Almeida, Guilherme |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
25 |
Should I learn from you? Seeing expectancy violations about action efficiency hinders social learning in infancy
|
Colomer, Marc |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
26 |
Social value at a distance: Higher identification with all of humanity is associated with reduced social discounting
|
Tuen, Young Ji |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
27 |
Strong perceptual consequences of low-level visual predictions: A new illusion
|
Jovanovic, Ljubica |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
28 |
Suppression-induced forgetting of motor sequences
|
Schmidt, Markus |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
29 |
Testing models of context-dependent outcome encoding in reinforcement learning
|
Hayes, William M. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
30 |
“The clothes (and the face) make the Starman”: Facial and clothing features shape self-other matching processes between human observers and a cartoon character
|
Welsh, Timothy N. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
31 |
The knowledge (“true belief”) error in 4- to 6-year-old children: When are agents aware of what they have in view?
|
Huemer, Michael |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
32 |
The language marker hypothesis
|
Hagoort, Peter |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
33 |
The role of reciprocity in dynamic interpersonal coordination of physiological rhythms
|
Konvalinka, Ivana |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
34 |
The temporal structure of parent talk to toddlers about objects
|
Slone, Lauren K. |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
35 |
Thinking more or thinking differently? Using drift-diffusion modeling to illuminate why accuracy prompts decrease misinformation sharing
|
Lin, Hause |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
36 |
Threat shapes visual context sensitivity selectively through low-spatial-frequency channels
|
Hu, Xuhua |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
37 |
Visual numerosity perception shows no advantage in real-world scenes compared to artificial displays
|
Odic, Darko |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
38 |
Visual working memory representations bias attention more when they are the target of an action plan
|
Trentin, Caterina |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
39 |
When more is less: Adding action effects to reduce crosstalk between concurrently performed tasks
|
Schacherer, Jonathan |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |
40 |
Why leading is (almost) as important as winning
|
Alves, Hans |
|
|
230 |
C |
p. |
artikel |